Revista Iberoamericana de Neuropsicología
Vol. 1, No. 2: 170-179, julio-diciembre 2018.
The neuropsychologist working in addiction:
What to know? Ten questions and answers
Antonio Verdejo-García, PhD1,2
1 Monash Institute of Cognitive and Clinical Neurosciences, Monash University
2 Turning Point Alcohol and Drug Centre
Correspondence:
Monash Institute of Cognitive and Clinical Neurosciences
18 Innovation Walk
Clayton VIC, 3800
Melbourne, Australia
The neuropsychologist working in addiction:
What to know? Ten questions and answers
Substance addiction is characterized by problems in controlling drug use, significant interference with other meaningful activities, and persistent use despite growing negative consequences. Psychoactive drugs have a strong impact on brain function and related consequences on thinking, emotion and behavior, and hence social and occupational functioning. Thus, this is an area of interest for neuropsychologists in terms of characterizing deficits, functional impact, and strategies for recovery or compensation. The aim of this review is to go over some of the fundamental questions and challenges that neuropsychologists working in this field often face. I approach this goal in the form of ten key questions and their corresponding answers, which are based on existing research and personal experience in the field. Questions and answers cover some of the fundamental aspects of drug-related neuropharmacological and behavioural effects, the neuropsychological assessment in the context of addiction, and the approaches to retraining and rehabilitation of deficits. I conclude by presenting a vision of the future for neuropsychological practice in the context of the addiction clinic.
Substance use disorders, or substance addictions, are characterized by problems in controlling drug use, significant interference with other meaningful activities and persistent use despite growing negative consequences1,2. Substance addictions are associated with significant medical and psychosocial harms and are a major contributor to the global burden of disease3. Psychoactive drugs, including alcohol, cannabis, stimulants and opioids, have a strong impact on brain function and related consequences on thinking, emotion and behavior, and hence social and occupational functioning4,5. Thus, substance addiction is an area of interest for neuropsychologists in terms of characterizing deficits, functional impact, and strategies for recovery or compensation. Moreover, the fact that there is diminished control and continued use despite negative consequences reflects alterations in executive control and decision-making, which seem to be at the core of the disorder6. Therefore, it is not surprising that substance addiction has become a fruitful area for neuropsychological research and practice, with growing scientific interest and implementation in research clinics and addiction treatment centers7,8. The aim of this review is to go over some of the fundamental questions and challenges that neuropsychologists working in this field have to face. I have done this in the form of ten key questions and their corresponding answers, which are based on existing research and personal experience in the field.
Yes. This seems like an obvious answer, but some researchers have challenged it9. The controversy may lie in the confusion between drug use and addiction. Many people use drugs experimentally and/or recreationally, and most won’t have cognitive deficits unless drug use escalates and related problems arise10. But addiction typically involves heavy drug use, difficulty to stop, and significant interference with daily life activities that keep our cognitive systems up to speed (e.g., studies, work, social relationships). We, and others, have shown that heavier drug use is dose-relatedly associated with poorer cognitive performance among people with different types of addiction11,12. Moreover, recent research has shown that people who meet cannabis addiction criteria have greater reductions in the brain’s gray matter than heavy cannabis users who do not meet these criteria13. This finding suggests that suffering addiction symptoms is linked to additional cognitive burden. Overall, there is compelling evidence from well-controlled studies and meta-analyses demonstrating that people with addiction typically have cognitive deficits14-16. Even so, there are, of course, some nuances of the issue. First, research suggests that the population is heterogeneous, and deficits may impact some (between 35 and 70% depending on addiction severity), but not all17. In addition, deficits are generally subtler than those found in populations with neurological disorders18, although similar to the ones observed in other populations with mental health problems, such as depression, bipolar or psychosis-related disorders19. In sum, we can assume that most people with addiction will have some cognitive deficits of at least mild to moderate severity.
Drugs of abuse produce supraphysiological stimulation (and hence dysregulation) of neurotransmitter systems that are pivotal for cognitive functioning. One of the most researched mechanisms is the neuroadaptation of dopaminergic corticolimbic circuits. Stimulant drugs such as cocaine or methamphetamine induce both transient and long-term neuroadaptations in striatal dopamine receptors20. Alcohol, cannabis and opioids also affect dopamine via indirect pathways21. Such neuroadaptations end up affecting the functioning of frontal-striatal systems, including those involved in executive functions, emotion regulation and decision-making22,23. But dopamine is only part of the story; stimulants have strong neuroadaptive effects on glutamate, noradrenaline and serotonin, alcohol on glutamate and GABA, and cannabis on the balance of several neurotransmitter systems that use negative feedback mechanisms linked to endocannabinoid modulators21,24. Moreover, most drugs mobilize second messenger systems that result in maladaptive changes in gene expression and neurotrophic cascades25. In addition to the well-known direct neuropharmacological effects of drugs, addiction (and its mental health strain) is associated with alterations in stress and pain systems, which have a considerable impact on attention, memory and executive functions26,27. Some drugs such as opioids do not have powerful neurotoxic effects, but they can impact cognitive function via dysregulation of stress and emotional systems. Altogether, there is compelling evidence that shows that drugs of abuse induce significant changes in brain function that are meaningfully associated with deterioration of cognitive systems involved in attention, memory, executive functions, emotion regulation and decision-making.
Drugs of abuse predominantly affect executive functions, decision-making and emotion regulation28,29. They also have negative effects on attention and memory, although in aspects that are at least partly related to executive control, such as memory coding, strategic retrieval, and sustained and selective attention30-32. An interesting point is the specific effects of different drugs. Systematic review and meta-analytic research has shown that addiction to stimulants such as cocaine and methamphetamine are linked to deficits in working memory, response inhibition, cognitive flexibility and decision-making15,17. Opioid addiction is associated with deficits in verbal episodic and working memory as well as fluency and decision-making14. Alcohol addiction relates to cognitive deficits across the board, spanning from basic abilities such as speed and language to attention and memory and more complex executive functions and decision-making16. Cannabis addiction, however, is linked to specific alterations in episodic memory33 although they can be short-lived34. MDMA (ecstasy) users also show discrete alterations of memory processes35,36. Most populations with addiction problems have deficits in emotion processing and regulation37 as well as social cognition and interaction problems38-40. The severity of cognitive deficits also differs as a function of the principal drug of choice. Deficits are typically of medium/large effect size in people with alcohol and stimulant addictions, whereas they are of small to medium effect size in the case of opioid and cannabis addictions. All of these patterns are aggravated in the context of polysubstance use, and thus people addicted to multiple substances show additive cognitive alterations12. Altogether, the domains of memory, executive functions, decision-making and social cognition are typically impaired, with medium effect size deficits, among people with addiction. Alcohol and stimulants such as cocaine and methamphetamine are linked to broader and more severe alterations, whereas opioids, cannabis and MDMA users have more specific alterations in decision-making and memory processes.
We should worry for at least three reasons. First, cognitive deficits can contribute to continuation and escalation of drug use in active users. Drugs can temporarily boost executive functions and emotion processing41,42, and thus people with cognitive deficits can be inclined to increase drug use to improve cognition and related outcomes (i.e., productivity, wellbeing). Second, cognitive deficits interfere with treatment efficacy. Addiction treatment involves cognitively-demanding psychotherapies, and longitudinal research has shown that people with poorer cognitive functioning are less likely to adhere to these interventions and more likely to dropout prematurely43,44. Third, there is a strong relationship between cognitive deficits and the risk of drug relapse. People with greater impulsivity and poorer decision-making skills are significantly more likely to relapse after treatment discharge45,46. Moreover, (dis)inhibition and impulsive decision-making also contribute to poorer recovery of quality of life47. In this regard, cognitive deficits can critically contribute to the chronic nature of addiction.
Cognitive deficits do improve with continuous abstinence, but it takes a long time, and we still do not know if recovery is complete48. Longitudinal research among people with cocaine and methamphetamine addiction have shown that six to twelve months of sobriety are associated with significant recovery of cognitive deficits, such that performance becomes very similar to that of healthy controls49,50. In people with alcohol addiction, over one year of sobriety is associated with normalization of most cognitive functions, with visual-motor skills being the most lingering deficits16,51. Therefore, we can confidently say that long-term abstinence pays off. But we also know that it is uncommon for people with addiction problems to completely abstain for such a long time. Unfortunately, cognitive recovery is not so apparent among people who reduce (but maintain) alcohol and drug use50,52. Another important consideration is that there are very few longitudinal studies, and some of them have not assessed some of the cognitive domains that are most critical for addiction, such as impulse control or decision-making16. Moreover, we need to consider that even normative recovery of cognitive functions might not be enough for some individuals, since (i) baseline cognitive skills failed to prevent onset of drug use in origin, and (ii) state-related fluctuations in mood and/or stress levels can return cognitive processes to a risky status53. Altogether, available research suggests that abstinence periods of over six months result in meaningful recovery of cognitive deficits among people with addiction, but additional support is needed to get more people over that line, and more comprehensive longitudinal studies are needed to determine if such recovery is complete.
People with addiction problems are likely to have lower education and socioeconomic levels than the general population or the populations typically assessed by neuropsychologists. Probably as a result, they also tend to show lower IQ levels, which can have a significant influence on their cognitive performance and particularly on executive function performance54. In addition, the prevalence of child trauma and neurodevelopmental disorders such as attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is greater in people with addiction than in the general population55. Proxys of childhood trauma have been associated with poorer performance in tests of executive function among people with cocaine addiction56. Similarly, the comorbidity between cocaine addiction and ADHD is associated with greater general cognitive deficits57. People with addiction are also more likely to suffer other mental health problems, including depression, bipolar disorder, psychosis-related disorders and personality disorders58,59. Comorbidity with psychosis is associated with additive deficits in a broad range of functions including speed, attention, memory and executive functions60. We and others have shown that the comorbidity with personality disorders is associated with cumulative deficits in working memory and response inhibition, as well as greater brain and behavioural alterations in emotion regulation compared to non-comorbid patients61-63. Although not many studies have examined cognition in the context of comorbidity with depression and bipolar disorders, available evidence suggest cumulative alterations in the executive function components of response inhibition and cognitive flexibility64. Finally, it is important to consider the presence of other medical comorbidities, such as HIV, hepatitis or alcoholism-related dementias, which have unique neuropsychological profiles associated with impairment of executive functions, episodic and working memory, processing speed and motor skills65.
The main challenges for neuropsychological assessment in the context of addiction are: controlling the effects of recent drug use; making an adequate estimation of premorbid characteristics versus addiction-related problems; achieving a good sampling/coverage of the most relevant cognitive domains; selecting appropriate tests for the population and the purpose of the assessment; and considering ecological validity17. With regard to recent drug use, we should be mindful about the cognitive-enhancing effects of certain drugs such as cocaine and amphetamines, as well as the cognitive-dampening effects of acute alcohol, cannabis or benzodiazepine intake. To make sure we are capturing long-term rather than acute effects, assessments should be scheduled at least 48 hours after last use, and ideally after two weeks (to rule out residual withdrawal effects). To discriminate between premorbid versus addiction related alterations, the interview should focus on some of the well-known antecedents of drug using behavior, including socioeconomic disadvantage, history of trauma, ADHD and psychosis-related and personality disorders (which precede or overlap with onset of drug use)66. Moreover, the assessment protocol needs to incorporate measures of IQ and lifetime drug use, to scrutinize the relationship between general intelligence and severity of drug use measures and cognitive performance. In terms of coverage, the assessment should focus on measuring episodic and working memory, sustained and selective attention, the different domains of executive function including fluency, response inhibition, cognitive flexibility and decision-making, and social cognition. Special emphasis should be placed on different aspects of impulsivity and decision-making, including reflection/planning, delay discounting, risk taking and effort-based decision-making, since they will be directly relevant to clinical outcomes43. Test selection needs to prioritize complex over simple tasks (e.g., California over Hopkins verbal learning test; Probabilistic over deterministic reversal learning tasks of flexibility), since deficits are not gross and will only be apparent in difficult tasks. Finally, in a still emerging field, it is important to demonstrate that the conclusions of our assessment will be relevant to explain difficulties in daily life. Therefore, we should prioritize ecologically valid tests, both in terms of predictive validity and similarity to real-life, relevant situations67.
Yes, but only when they are targeted and tailored to the specific deficits of the population. At least three cognitive training/rehabilitation strategies have been shown to be effective in restoring cognitive deficits and/or improving clinical outcomes in patients with addiction. Approach Bias Modification, which uses computerized training to tame approach biases towards alcohol cues and promote approach biases towards non-alcoholic beverages (e.g., juices, sodas), has shown to reduce relapse rates in numerous studies68,69,70. However, it is unclear if the training can be successfully generalized to other forms of substance addiction, since most drugs (unlike alcohol) do not have straightforward alternative rewarding stimuli. Computerized Working Memory Training has been shown to reduce alcohol use in problem drinkers71, illicit drug use in methadone maintenance patients72 and impulsivity levels in people with methamphetamine addiction73 (but see74). Using a more holistic, compensatory-based approach, Goal Management Training (GMT), which was originally designed for executive dysfunction in neurological patients, has also obtained very promising results. GMT can decrease impulsivity and improve planning and decision-making in patients with polysubstance use75,76 and patients with methamphetamine addiction and HIV77. Altogether, there is growing, promising evidence on the efficacy of cognitive training and rehabilitation as an adjunctive strategy for the treatment of addiction, but there is also a need for better-controlled trials and examination of moderators and mediators of training/rehabilitation effects.
We have shown that Approach Bias Modification is feasible and effective as early as during the detoxification phase once acute withdrawal symptoms are medically controlled69. In fact, it is possible that cognitive training during detoxification can take advantage of the neuroplasticity processes that accompany early abstinence78. The application of more intensive cognitive rehabilitation strategies, such as Goal Management Training, requires more time and engagement from participants, and hence it can be better suited for dishabituation after acute and residual withdrawal effects have subsided. Therefore, it is theoretically plausible to link up both approaches, using computerized cognitive training to strengthen prerequisite cognitive processes during the early detoxification stage (e.g., attention, working memory), and then capitalize on such improvements to facilitate delivery of Goal Management Training or other intensive interventions to consolidate meta-cognitive strategies and apply them to real-life scenarios. Future studies will need to assess the validity of this concept and the potential efficacy of the combination. Another promising approach is overlapping cognitive training/rehabilitation interventions with other non-neuropsychological therapies that can synergize their effects. For example, we have applied Goal Management Training in combination with mindfulness, using the latter to smooth the transition between identification of impulses and the development of a goal-focused meta-cognitive approach75,76. Cognitive training interventions can also benefit from the neuroplasticity changes that can be achieved via physical exercise training. For example, a recent study has shown that eight weeks of aerobic exercise training increased the availability of dopaminergic receptors in the striatum79. However, more research is needed to establish what is the right blend, timing and intensity of these combined interventions.
The future should bring greater and improved utilization of neuropsychologists in addiction treatment centers. After two decades of solid research on the characterization of deficits, the addiction clinic should embrace this knowledge, and neuropsychologists need to be ready to implement it67. There are several models to achieve this goal. One is embedding a neuropsychology service in existing addition treatment centers, taking advantage of a pre-existing structure and a comprehensive duty of care. Another possibility is creating specialized addiction-neuropsychology clinics, which then use other complementary external services. The future will also bring more consensus and evidence-based practice on neuropsychological assessments and interventions tailored to populations with substance addictions7,80. The format of assessments and interventions will probably change. Assessments will become more portable and engaging using digital technology. Additionally, interventions will likely be more sophisticated, with greater personalization and dynamic adjustments as a function of therapeutic progress. The essence remains, however, that addiction is inherently a disorder of executive control and decision-making, and thus will necessitate better assessment and intervention tools to profile and amend executive and decision-making deficits.
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.
- World Health Organization. ICD-11 Beta draft; 2011; [cited 2017Nov3]. Available from: https://icd.who.int/dev11/l-m/en
- Degenhardt L, Whiteford HA, Ferrari AJ, Baxter AJ, Charlson FJ, Hall WD, et al. Global burden of disease attributable to illicit drug use and dependence: findings from the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. The Lancet. 2013;382(9904):1564–74.
- Volkow ND, Baler RD, Goldstein RZ. Addiction: Pulling at the Neural Threads of Social Behaviors. Neuron. 2011;69(4):599–602.
- Volkow ND, Morales M. The Brain on Drugs: From Reward to Addiction. Cell. 2015;162(4):712–25.
- Bechara A. Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: a neurocognitive perspective. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8(11):1458–63.
- Franken IH, Wetering BJVD. Bridging the gap between the neurocognitive lab and the addiction clinic. Addictive Behaviors. 2015;44:108–14.
- Rezapour T, Devito EE, Sofuoglu M, Ekhtiari H. Perspectives on neurocognitive rehabilitation as an adjunct treatment for addictive disorders. Progress in Brain Research Neuroscience for Addiction Medicine: From Prevention to Rehabilitation – Methods and Interventions. 2016;345–69.
- Lewis M. Addiction and the Brain: Development, Not Disease. Neuroethics. 2017;10(1):7-18.
- Verdejo-García A, Sánchez-Fernández MDM, Alonso-Maroto LM, Fernández-Calderón F, Perales JC, Lozano Ó, et al. Impulsivity and executive functions in polysubstance-using rave attenders. Psychopharmacology. 2010;210(3):377–92.
- Dean AC, Groman SM, Morales AM, London ED. An Evaluation of the Evidence that Methamphetamine Abuse Causes Cognitive Decline in Humans. 2013 Jan;38(2):259-74.
- Fernández-Serrano MJ, Pérez-García M, Verdejo-García A. What are the specific vs. generalized effects of drugs of abuse on neuropsychological performance? Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2011;35(3):377–406.
- Chye Y, Suo C, Yücel M, Ouden LD, Solowij N, Lorenzetti V. Cannabis-related hippocampal volumetric abnormalities specific to subregions in dependent users. Psychopharmacology. 2017;234(14):2149–57.
- Baldacchino A, Balfour D, Passetti F, Humphris G, Matthews K. Neuropsychological consequences of chronic opioid use: A quantitative review and meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2012;36(9):2056–68.
- Jovanovski D, Erb S, Zakzanis KK. Neurocognitive Deficits in Cocaine Users: A Quantitative Review of the Evidence. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2005;27(2):189–204.
- Stavro K, Pelletier J, Potvin S. Widespread and sustained cognitive deficits in alcoholism: a meta-analysis. Addiction Biology. 2013 Mar;18(2):203-13.
- Fernández-Serrano MJ, Pérez-García M, Perales JC, Verdejo-García A. Prevalence of executive dysfunction in cocaine, heroin and alcohol users enrolled in therapeutic communities. European Journal of Pharmacology. 2010;626(1):104–12.
- Caracuel A, Verdejogarcia A, Vilarlopez R, Perezgarcia M, Salinas I, Cuberos G, et al. Frontal behavioral and emotional symptoms in Spanish individuals with acquired brain injury and substance use disorders. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2008;23(4):447–54.
- Yücel M, Lubman DI, Solowij N, Brewer WJ. Understanding Drug Addiction: A Neuropsychological Perspective. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. 2007;41(12):957–68.
- London ED, Kohno M, Morales AM, Ballard ME. Chronic methamphetamine abuse and corticostriatal deficits revealed by neuroimaging. Brain Research. 2015;1628:174–85.
- Cami J, Farre M. Drug addiction. New England Journal of Medicine. 2003;349(10): 975-986.
- Ghahremani DG, Lee B, Robertson CL, Tabibnia G, Morgan AT, Shetler ND, et al. Striatal Dopamine D2/D3 Receptors Mediate Response Inhibition and Related Activity in Frontostriatal Neural Circuitry in Humans. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32(21):7316–24.
- Tabibnia G, Monterosso JR, Baicy K, Aron AR, Poldrack RA, Chakrapani S, et al. Different Forms of Self-Control Share a Neurocognitive Substrate. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31(13):4805–10.
- Maldonado R, Valverde O, Berrendero F. Involvement of the endocannabinoid system in drug addiction. Trends in Neurosciences. 2006 Apr;29(4):225-32.
- Chao J, Nestler E.J. Molecular neurobiology of drug addiction. Annual Review of Medicine. 2004;55:113-132.
- Elman I, Borsook D. Common Brain Mechanisms of Chronic Pain and Addiction. Neuron. 2016;89(1):11–36.
- Koob GF, Volkow ND. Neurobiology of addiction: a neurocircuitry analysis. The Lancet Psychiatry. 2016;3(8):760–73.
- Verdejo-García A, Bechara A. A somatic marker theory of addiction. 2009;56:48–62.
- Verdejo-García A, Bechara A, Recknor EC, Pérez-García M. Executive dysfunction in substance dependent individuals during drug use and abstinence: An examination of the behavioral, cognitive and emotional correlates of addiction. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2006;12(03):405-415.
- Quednow BB, Jessen F, Kühn K-U, Maier W, Daum I, Wagner M. Memory deficits in abstinent MDMA (ecstasy) users: neuropsychological evidence of frontal dysfunction. Journal of Psychopharmacology. 2006 May;20(3):373-84.
- Vilar-López R, Takagi M, Lubman DI, Cotton SM, Bora E, Verdejo-García A, et al. The Effects of Inhalant Misuse on Attentional Networks. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2013;38(2):126–36.
- Wesley MJ, Bickel WK. Remember the Future II: Meta-analyses and Functional Overlap of Working Memory and Delay Discounting. Biological Psychiatry. 2014;75(6):435–48.
- Grant I, Gonzalez R, Carey CL, Natarajan L, Wolfson T. Non-acute (residual) neurocognitive effects of cannabis use: A meta-analytic study. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2003;9(05):679-689.
- Schreiner AM, Dunn ME. Residual effects of cannabis use on neurocognitive performance after prolonged abstinence: A meta-analysis. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2012;20(5):420–9.
- Wunderli MD, Vonmoos M, Fürst M, Schädelin K, Kraemer T, Baumgartner MR, et al. Discrete memory impairments in largely pure chronic users of MDMA. European Neuropsychopharmacology. 2017;27(10):987–99.
- Zakzanis KK, Campbell Z, Jovanovski D. The neuropsychology of ecstasy (MDMA) use: a quantitative review. Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experimental. 2007;22(7):427–35.
- Castellano F, Bartoli F, Crocamo C, Gamba G, Tremolada M, Santambrogio J, et al. Facial emotion recognition in alcohol and substance use disorders: A meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2015;59:147–54.
- Bora E, Zorlu N. Social cognition in alcohol use disorder: a meta-analysis. Addiction. 2017 Jan;112(1):40-48.
- Preller KH, Herdener M, Schilbach L, Stämpfli P, Hulka LM, Vonmoos M, Ingold N, Vogeley K, Tobler PN, Seifritz E, Quednow BB. Functional changes of the reward system underlie blunted response to social gaze in cocaine users. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U S A. 2014 Feb 18;111(7):2842-7.
- Verdejo-Garcia A. Social cognition in cocaine addiction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U S A. 2014 Feb 18;111(7):2406-7.
- Arcos FAD, Verdejo-García A, Ceverino A, Montañez-Pareja M, López-Juárez E, Sánchez-Barrera M, et al. Dysregulation of emotional response in current and abstinent heroin users: negative heightening and positive blunting. 2008Nov;198(2):159–66.
- Garavan H1, Kaufman JN, Hester R. Acute effects of cocaine on the neurobiology of cognitive control. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Science. 2008 Oct 12;363(1507):3267-76.
- Domínguez-Salas S, Díaz-Batanero C, Lozano-Rojas OM, Verdejo-García A. Impact of general cognition and executive function deficits on addiction treatment outcomes: Systematic review and discussion of neurocognitive pathways. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2016 Dec;71:772-801.
- Stevens L, Verdejo-García A, Goudriaan AE, Roeyers H, Dom G, Vanderplasschen W. Impulsivity as a vulnerability factor for poor addiction treatment outcomes: a review of neurocognitive findings among individuals with substance use disorders. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment. 2014 Jul;47(1):58-72.
- Stevens L, Goudriaan AE, Verdejo-Garcia A, Dom G, Roeyers H, Vanderplasschen W. Impulsive choice predicts short-term relapse in substance-dependent individuals attending an in-patient detoxification programme. Psychological Medicine. 2015 Jul;45(10):2083-93.
- Verdejo-Garcia A, Albein-Urios N, Martinez-Gonzalez JM, Civit E, de la Torre R, Lozano O. Decision-making impairment predicts 3-month hair-indexed cocaine relapse. Psychopharmacology. 2014 Oct;231(21):4179-87.
- Rubenis AJ, Fitzpatrick RE, Lubman DI, Verdejo-Garcia A. Impulsivity predicts poorer improvement in quality of life during early treatment for people with methamphetamine dependence. Addiction. 2018 Apr;113(4):668-676.
- Schulte MH, Cousijn J, den Uyl TE, Goudriaan AE, van den Brink W, Veltman DJ, et al. Recovery of neurocognitive functions following sustained abstinence after substance dependence and implications for treatment. Clinical Psychology Review. 2014 Nov;34(7):531-50.
- Iudicello JE, Woods SP, Vigil O, Scott JC, Cherner M, Heaton RK, et al. Longer term improvement in neurocognitive functioning and affective distress among methamphetamine users who achieve stable abstinence. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2010 Aug;32(7):704-18.
- Vonmoos M, Hulka LM, Preller KH, Minder F, Baumgartner MR, Quednow BB. Cognitive impairment in cocaine users is drug-induced but partially reversible: evidence from a longitudinal study. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014 Aug;39(9):2200-10.
- Fein G, Torres J, Price LJ, Di Sclafani V. Cognitive performance in long-term abstinent alcoholic individuals. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2006 Sep;30(9):1538-44.
- de Sola S, Tarancón T, Peña-Casanova J, Espadaler JM, Langohr K, Poudevida S. Auditory event-related potentials (P3) and cognitive performance in recreational ecstasy polydrug users: evidence from a 12-month longitudinal study. Psychopharmacology. 2008 Oct;200(3):425-37.
- Park H, Lee D, Chey J. Stress enhances model-free reinforcement learning only after negative outcome. PLoS One. 2017 Jul 19;12(7):e0180588.
- Meier MH, Caspi A, Danese A, Fisher HL, Houts R, Arseneault L, et al. Associations between adolescent cannabis use and neuropsychological decline: a longitudinal co-twin control study. Addiction. 2018 Feb;113(2):257-265.
- Vogel T, Dom G, van de Glind G, Studer J, Gmel G, Strik W, et al. Is attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder among men associated with initiation or escalation of substance use at 15-month follow-up? A longitudinal study involving young Swiss men. Addiction. 2016 Oct;111(10):1867-78.
- Viola TW, Tractenberg SG, Pezzi JC, Kristensen CH, Grassi-Oliveira R. Childhood physical neglect associated with executive functions impairments in crack cocaine-dependent women. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2013 Sep 1;132(1-2):271-6.
- Wunderli MD, Vonmoos M, Niedecker SM, Hulka LM, Preller KH, Baumgartner MR, et al. Cognitive and emotional impairments in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and cocaine use. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2016 Jun 1;163:92-9.
- Grant BF, Stinson FS, Dawson DA, Chou SP, Ruan WJ, Pickering RP. Co-occurrence of 12-month alcohol and drug use disorders and personality disorders in the United States: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2004 Apr;61(4):361-8.
- Grant BF, Stinson FS, Dawson DA, Chou SP, Dufour MC, Compton W, et al. Prevalence and co-occurrence of substance use disorders and independent mood and anxiety disorders: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2004 Aug;61(8):807-16.
- Chen YC, Wang LJ, Lin SK, Chen CK. Profiles of Methamphetamine Users: Comparison of Those With or Without Concomitant Ketamine Use. Substance Use & Misuse. 2015;50(14):1778-85.
- Albein-Urios N, Martinez-Gonzalez JM, Lozano-Rojas O, Verdejo-Garcia A. Executive functions in cocaine-dependent patients with Cluster B and Cluster C personality disorders. Neuropsychology. 2014 Jan;28(1):84-90.
- Albein-Urios N1, Verdejo-Román J, Soriano-Mas C, Asensio S, Martínez-González JM, Verdejo-García A. Cocaine users with comorbid Cluster B personality disorders show dysfunctional brain activation and connectivity in the emotional regulation networks during negative emotion maintenance and reappraisal. European Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013 Dec;23(12):1698-707.
- Dom G, De Wilde B, Hulstijn W, van den Brink W, Sabbe B. Behavioural aspects of impulsivity in alcoholics with and without a cluster-B personality disorder. Alcohol and Alcoholism. 2006 Jul-Aug;41(4):412-20.
- Duijkers JC, Vissers CT, Egger JI. Unraveling Executive Functioning in Dual Diagnosis. Frontiers in Psychology. 2016 Jun 28;7:979.
- Loftis JM, Huckans M, Weber E, Woods SP. Infectious diseases and substance use disorder comorbidity. In Allen DN, Woods SP. Neuropsychological aspects of substance use disorders. New York: Oxford University Press; 2014.
- Paus T, Keshavan M, Giedd JN. Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence? Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2008 Dec;9(12):947-57.
- Verdejo-Garcia A. Neuroclinical Assessment of Addiction Needs to Incorporate Decision-Making Measures and Ecological Validity. Biological Psychiatry. 2017 Apr 1;81(7):e53-e54.
- Eberl C, Wiers RW, Pawelczack S, Rinck M, Becker ES, Lindenmeyer J. Approach bias modification in alcohol dependence: do clinical effects replicate and for whom does it work best? Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2013 Apr;4:38-51.
- Manning V, Staiger PK, Hall K, Garfield JB, Flaks G, Leung D, et al. Cognitive Bias Modification Training During Inpatient Alcohol Detoxification Reduces Early Relapse: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2016Apr;40(9):2011–9.
- Wiers RW, Eberl C, Rinck M, Becker ES, Lindenmeyer J. Retraining automatic action tendencies changes alcoholic patients’ approach bias for alcohol and improves treatment outcome. Psychological Science. 2011 Apr;22(4):490-7.
- Houben K1, Wiers RW, Jansen A. Getting a grip on drinking behavior: training working memory to reduce alcohol abuse. Psychological Science. 2011 Jul;22(7):968-75.
- Rass O, Schacht RL, Buckheit K, Johnson MW, Strain EC, Mintzer MZ. A randomized controlled trial of the effects of working memory training in methadone maintenance patients. Drug and Alcohol Dependende. 2015 Nov 1;156:38-46.
- Bickel WK, Yi R, Landes RD, Hill PF, Baxter C. Remember the future: working memory training decreases delay discounting among stimulant addicts. Biological Psychiatry. 2011 Feb 1;69(3):260-5.
- Wanmaker S, Leijdesdorff SMJ, Geraerts E, van de Wetering BJM, Renkema PJ, Franken IHA. The efficacy of a working memory training in substance use patients: A randomized double-blind placebo-controlled clinical trial. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2018 Jun;40(5):473-486.
- Alfonso JP, Caracuel A, Delgado-Pastor LC, Verdejo-García A. Combined Goal Management Training and Mindfulness meditation improve executive functions and decision-making performance in abstinent polysubstance abusers. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2011 Aug 1;117(1):78-81.
- Valls-Serrano C, Caracuel A, Verdejo-Garcia A. Goal Management Training and Mindfulness Meditation improve executive functions and transfer to ecological tasks of daily life in polysubstance users enrolled in therapeutic community treatment. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2016 Aug 1;165:9-14.
- Casaletto KB, Moore DJ, Woods SP, Umlauf A, Scott JC, Heaton RK. Abbreviated Goal Management Training Shows Preliminary Evidence as a Neurorehabilitation Tool for HIV-associated Neurocognitive Disorders among Substance Users. The Clinical Neuropsychologist. 2016;30(1):107-30.
- van Eijk J, Demirakca T, Frischknecht U, Hermann D, Mann K, Ende G. Rapid partial regeneration of brain volume during the first 14 days of abstinence from alcohol. Alcoholism Clinical and Experimental Research. 2013 Jan;37(1):67-74.
- Robertson CL, Ishibashi K, Chudzynski J, Mooney LJ, Rawson RA, Dolezal BA, et al. Effect of Exercise Training on Striatal Dopamine D2/D3 Receptors in Methamphetamine Users during Behavioral Treatment. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016 May;41(6):1629-36.
- Kwako LE, Momenan R, Litten RZ, Koob GF, Goldman D. Addictions Neuroclinical Assessment: A Neuroscience-Based Framework for Addictive Disorders. Biological Psychiatry. 2016 Aug 1;80(3):179-89.